Dei'ah veDibur - Information & Insight
  

A Window into the Chareidi World

11 Tammuz 5764 - June 30, 2004 | Mordecai Plaut, director Published Weekly
NEWS

OPINION
& COMMENT

OBSERVATIONS

HOME
& FAMILY

IN-DEPTH
FEATURES

VAAD HORABBONIM HAOLAMI LEINYONEI GIYUR

TOPICS IN THE NEWS

HOMEPAGE

 

Produced and housed by
Shema Yisrael Torah Network
Shema Yisrael Torah Network

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

Home and Family


Can He See?
by A. Ross

It is impossible to state that sight is more miraculous than any other part of our wonderful bodies, but it is certainly a very complicated process. The lens automatically absorbs what it sees, but the brain feeds us far more detail. By looking at an object, we know the size, the color, the feel of the thing, where it is situated, what its purpose is etc. If we see a car from close up and another from afar, the latter will appear much smaller, yet we know that the two vehicles are more or less the same size. The brain with its cognitive experience automatically adjusts our perception.

What about babies without any cognitive experience? Although the optic nerve begins to develop in a three-week-old fetus, the whole visual system is not complete till after birth. How does this affect a newborn baby? What does he see, and does he see things differently from adults? How many mothers of newborns ask almost immediately, "Is everything alright? Can he see?"

The pupils of the eye contract or dilate according to the amount of light to which they are exposed. This reflex functions even in prematurely born babies, which proves that they are sensitive to various strengths of light at birth. Although this reflex works relatively sluggishly in babies, if we move an object slowly in front of a baby who is only a few days old, he will focus and be able to follow it with his eyes. The movements of the eye may be slightly jerky, but he can definitely see something. Interestingly, the optimum distance for this experiment is about eight or nine inches away from his face, which is about the distance he is away from his mother's eyes as she nurses him. The eyes are not yet synchronized, which frequently gives concerned parents the idea that the baby has a squint. A baby as young as five days old might interrupt his nursing for a moment to follow a shaft of light.

After the baby has seen a particular object several times, he may not focus on it any more, and we might think he is tired, or cannot focus at all times. Amazingly, if you show this two- week-old an entirely different object, his attention will be riveted again. He was just `bored' with the original one, or to put it more professionally, he had become acclimatized; he has lost interest. Furthermore, he has proven that he can discrimate between the two objects.

Researchers used this information to find out whether, already at the age of two weeks, babies showed preference to any particular type of object. They discovered that contours, i.e., the size of a particular shape, riveted their attention. For instance, after focusing on a large black square painted on a white background, the babies would keep looking at the dividing line between the black and the white. A chessboard was found to be particularly fascinating to babies. So do we surround their cribs with chessboards?

Having discovered this ability, nobody has yet found out why this is so, and the babies are not about to enlighten us. So how can we make use of this information to aid the child's development? Human faces have many contours. The hairline above the forehead, the eyebrows and the lips give plenty of scope to the babies' inherent preferences, and help them perfect their lines of vision. In other words, just looking at the faces of people around him will help a baby develop. Much has been written about the developing eyesight, and experiments which have been carried out.

Parents could try the following on a very tiny baby. Take three similar objects: oval or roundish like the human face, perhaps a paper plate. On one, draw a face in bold black felt pen. Scribble indiscrimate lines on the second and color the third one black all over. Arrange them in a row within the baby's field of vision (after he has been fed and when he is feeling content!) and in all probability, he will focus far longer and more frequently on the human face! Don't write to YATED if your baby did not pass the test!

Between four and eight weeks old, babies show marked preference for stripes, for instance, concentric circles getting increasingly narrower and smaller towards the center, whereas monkeys and some types of bird recoil from these shapes. One theory is that it is similar to human eyes. By this time, the baby will be able to focus on the line between the hair and the forehead, and begin to make eye contact with his mother or other carer. This gives the carer the feeling that he recognizes her and strengthens the emotional bond between them.

Abut twenty years ago, some extensive research was carried out on babies between the age of sixteen to thirty weeks. They found that more complicated pictures were of more interest to their `subjects.' For instance, a picture of three squares was more interesting than one of two. Or three different-colored circles drew their attention more than three circles colored the same.

Babies cannot count, yet unfailingly, in experiments carried out with different objectts, they were more interested in the greater number of objects. When the outlines were too large, or when there were too many small shapes or objects, the babies lost interest and did not do more than glance at them. Older children and even adults prefer to concentrate on up to a maximum of five objects at a time.

At about four months, the baby's sight and cognition is so well developed that when shown a whole series of pictures of the human face, he will focus far longer on those which seem `real,' on those which look more like people and less like drawings.

We are all aware of the fact that everything we see influences our lives. If a baby is exposed to undesirable pictures and sights, claims Rav Wolbe, this will influence the child adversely. He goes so far as to say that exposure to pictures of non-kosher animals, even when speaking of tiny babies, will also have a detrimental effect. Cuddly toys should preferably be sheep or other kosher animals. Most families do have a cherished teddy bear, yet perhaps we should create a new trend?

Do babies see things in two dimensions or three? Various sophisticated experiments have been carried out which show that babies are born with spacial awareness. Their inability to grasp toys at an early age is not because they cannot see the object and the distance they would have to reach out, but because they do not yet have the muscular ability. Moreover, older babies will raise a hand to protect themselves from an oncoming object which seems threatening, and little babies will cry or show other signs of distress.

By measuring among other things the baby's heartbeat and how vigorously he sucks his pacifier, researchers have obtained fairly accurate information about the early development and competence of a baby. While rejoicing in yet another miracle of creation, we should also appreciate the knowledge and ability Hashem has given scientists to carry out this research!

 

All material on this site is copyrighted and its use is restricted.
Click here for conditions of use.